-
Notifications
You must be signed in to change notification settings - Fork 21
/
Copy pathCreatine.page
355 lines (261 loc) · 28.9 KB
/
Creatine.page
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
61
62
63
64
65
66
67
68
69
70
71
72
73
74
75
76
77
78
79
80
81
82
83
84
85
86
87
88
89
90
91
92
93
94
95
96
97
98
99
100
101
102
103
104
105
106
107
108
109
110
111
112
113
114
115
116
117
118
119
120
121
122
123
124
125
126
127
128
129
130
131
132
133
134
135
136
137
138
139
140
141
142
143
144
145
146
147
148
149
150
151
152
153
154
155
156
157
158
159
160
161
162
163
164
165
166
167
168
169
170
171
172
173
174
175
176
177
178
179
180
181
182
183
184
185
186
187
188
189
190
191
192
193
194
195
196
197
198
199
200
201
202
203
204
205
206
207
208
209
210
211
212
213
214
215
216
217
218
219
220
221
222
223
224
225
226
227
228
229
230
231
232
233
234
235
236
237
238
239
240
241
242
243
244
245
246
247
248
249
250
251
252
253
254
255
256
257
258
259
260
261
262
263
264
265
266
267
268
269
270
271
272
273
274
275
276
277
278
279
280
281
282
283
284
285
286
287
288
289
290
291
292
293
294
295
296
297
298
299
300
301
302
303
304
305
306
307
308
309
310
311
312
313
314
315
316
317
318
319
320
321
322
323
324
325
326
327
328
329
330
331
332
333
334
335
336
337
338
339
340
341
342
343
344
345
346
347
348
349
350
351
352
353
354
355
---
title: Creatine Meta-analysis
description: Does creatine increase IQ?
created: 6 Sep 2013
tags: psychology, meta-analysis
status: notes
belief: possible
...
> I attempt to meta-analyze conflicting studies about the cognitive benefits of creatine supplementation. The wide variety of psychological measures hampers any aggregation. 3 studies measured IQ and turn in a positive result, but suggestive of vegetarianism causing half the benefit.
[Creatine](!Wikipedia) is a chemical found throughout the body in a number of roles; it is most famous for its presence in muscles and enabling greater exertion, but it also plays a role in the nervous system. Some small psychology experiments in healthy adults have found cognitive benefits to supplementation but others disagree ([Wikipedia](!Wikipedia "Creatine#Improved cognitive ability"), [Examine.com](http://examine.com/supplements/Creatine/#summary6-12)), and these differences may be due to covariates like being vegetarian & hence creatine-deficient.
When small studies conflict, one way to get answers is to try to meta-analyze them into a single more robust summary. In particular, I am interested in whether creatine supplementation increases IQ. One problem here is that the studies may not use enough of the same measures to include in the same meta-analysis; besides that, the studies are likely severely underpowered to detect plausible effects: IQ increases have been often claimed, but have rarely panned out (see, for example, [dual n-back](DNB meta-analysis)) and "extraordinary claims require extraordinary proof".
# Background
While creatine is famous for its athletic uses^[The author agrees with creatine's utility for athletics, having [used it](Nootropics#creatine) for that purpose himself.], there is also biological evidence suggesting creatine is involved in mental performance, serving as a fast source of energy, creatine-related retardation & disability, correlates between meat-eating and performance etc.
For one survey, and a more detailed discussion of the rationale for expecting creatine to help, see [Littleton 2013](/docs/creatine/2013-littleton.pdf "Cognitive performance in relation to persistence hunting and meat consumption").
An interesting longitudinal correlation between creatine levels and later increased salary is found in [Böckerman et al 2014](http://ftp.iza.org/dp8029.pdf "Biomarkers and Long-term Labour Market Outcomes: The Case of Creatine").
Experimentally, creatine turns out to boost mental performance in some circumstances (eg. [Ling et al 2009](http://jtoomim.org/files/Ling_2009-Cognitive_effects_of_creatine_ethyl_ester_supplementation.pdf "Cognitive effects of creatine ethyl ester supplementation") saw the creatine group post-score 4 points higher than controls, or [Watanabe 2002](/docs/creatine/2002-watanabe.pdf "Effects of creatine on mental fatigue and cerebral oxygenation")'s less oxygen use during mental arithmetic; Jonathan Toomim recommends it highly, claiming that "I'm more confident that I've noticed effects [on mental performance] of creatine than of DnB.")
These results are a little mixed. There are studies showing benefits in:
1. Vegetarians ([Rae 2003](http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC1691485/pdf/14561278.pdf "Oral creatine monohydrate supplementation improves brain performance: a double-blind, placebo-controlled, cross-over trial"))
2. the sleep-deprived ([McMorris 2006](/docs/creatine/2006-mcmorris.pdf "Effect of creatine supplementation and sleep deprivation, with mild exercise, on cognitive and psychomotor performance, mood state, and plasma concentrations of catecholamines and cortisol"); [McMorris 2007](/docs/creatine/2007-mcmorris-1.pdf "Creatine supplementation, sleep deprivation, cortisol, melatonin and behavior"))
3. the elderly ([McMorris et al 2007](/docs/creatine/2007-mcmorris-2.pdf "Creatine supplementation and cognitive performance in elderly individuals"))
However, [Rawson et al 2008](/docs/creatine/2008-rawson.pdf "Creatine supplementation does not improve cognitive function in young adults") is a broad null result for healthy omnivores, who are probably most of the readers of this FAQ. ([Jonathan Toomim](https://groups.google.com/group/brain-training/msg/5d5ca4de825f7187) has criticized Rawson et al 2008 as statistically weak and using a possibly not sensitive test of mental performance; others have pointed out Rawson et al 2008 administered a total $(73.8 \times 0.03) \times (6 \times 7) = \approx 93g$ of creatine, which is less than half that of Rae 2003 and 16% less than Ling et al 2009.)
# Search
Besides the studies I already knew about through discussion, my own research, Wikipedia, or Examine, I also did several searches for additional studies modeled on the search query `creatine AND (IQ OR intelligence OR "Raven's") -mutagen` ('IQ' turns out to be the name of a mutagen which appears in many contexts with [creatine kinase](!Wikipedia)):
- [Google](https://encrypted.google.com/search?q=creatine%20AND%20%28IQ%20OR%20intelligence%20OR%20%22Raven%27s%22%29%20-mutagen): up to pg10 of results
- [Google Scholar](http://scholar.google.com/scholar?q=creatine+AND+%28IQ+OR+intelligence+OR+%22Raven%27s%22%29+-mutagen): up to pg41
- [Pubmed](https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/?term=%28%22creatine%22[MeSH%20Terms]%20OR%20%22creatine%22[All%20Fields]%29%20AND%20%28IQ[All%20Fields]%20OR%20%28%22intelligence%22[MeSH%20Terms]%20OR%20%22intelligence%22[All%20Fields]%29%20OR%20%22Raven%27s%22[All%20Fields]%29%20AND%20%22humans%22[MeSH%20Terms]&cmd=DetailsSearch): 129 results, reviewed all
- [APA PsycNET](http://psycnet.apa.org/index.cfm?fa=search.searchResults): for just `creatine` (more complex queries are apparently not supported), 4 hits (none relevant)
- ProQuest Dissertations & Theses Full Text and ERIC `all(creatine) AND (all(intelligence) OR all(IQ))`: 2 hits
I then set up a Google Alert, a Google Scholar alert, and a Pubmed alert for those searches in case any new studies come out.
# Overview
The searches & alerts yielded the following potentially useful studies:
- [Watanabe 2002](/docs/creatine/2002-watanabe.pdf "Effects of creatine on mental fatigue and cerebral oxygenation")
- [Rae 2003](http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC1691485/pdf/14561278.pdf "Oral creatine monohydrate supplementation improves brain performance: a double-blind, placebo-controlled, cross-over trial")
- [McMorris 2006](/docs/creatine/2006-mcmorris.pdf "Effect of creatine supplementation and sleep deprivation, with mild exercise, on cognitive and psychomotor performance, mood state, and plasma concentrations of catecholamines and cortisol")
- [McMorris 2007](/docs/creatine/2007-mcmorris-1.pdf "Creatine supplementation, sleep deprivation, cortisol, melatonin and behavior")
- [McMorris et al 2007](/docs/creatine/2007-mcmorris-2.pdf "Creatine supplementation and cognitive performance in elderly individuals")
- [Gastner et al 2007](http://www.google.com/patents/DE102007030495A1 "Use of creatine containing preparation e.g. for improving memory, retentivity, long-term memory and for preventing mental fatigue condition, comprising e.g. Ginkgo biloba, ginseng and niacin")
- [Rawson et al 2008](/docs/creatine/2008-rawson.pdf "Creatine supplementation does not improve cognitive function in young adults")
- [Ling et al 2009](http://jtoomim.org/files/Ling_2009-Cognitive_effects_of_creatine_ethyl_ester_supplementation.pdf "Cognitive effects of creatine ethyl ester supplementation")
- [Hammett et al 2010](http://www.researchgate.net/publication/44694983_Dietary_supplementation_of_creatine_monohydrate_reduces_the_human_fMRI_BOLD_signal/file/9fcfd50bb2b9e2fba1.pdf "Dietary supplementation of creatine monohydrate reduces the human fMRI BOLD signal")
- [Benton & Donohoe 2011](http://img2.timg.co.il/forums/1_161120651.pdf "The influence of creatine supplementation on the cognitive functioning of vegetarians and omnivores")
- [Alves et al 2013](http://www.plosone.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pone.0076301 "Creatine Supplementation Associated or Not with Strength Training upon Emotional and Cognitive Measures in Older Women: A Randomized Double-Blind Study")
These use a variety of psychological measures:
Study Test
------------------------- ---------------------
Watanabe 2002 Uchida-Kraepelin serial calculation test
Rae 2003 Raven's Advanced Progressive Matrices; Backward Digit Span
McMorris 2006 Random movement generation test; forward verbal recall; backwards verbal recall; 4-choice visual reaction time test; Hemmings mood state inventory
McMorris 2007 Random number generation task; number recall test; four-choice visual reaction time test; Hemmings mood state inventory; NASA-TLX effort sub-scale
McMorris et al 2007 Random number generation task; forward verbal recall; backwards verbal recall; forward Corsi Block Tapping test; backward Corsi Block Tapping test; long-term memory test
Gastner et al 2007 Raven's Advanced Progressive Matrices; Uchida-Kraepelin serial calculation test; Backward Digit Span
Rawson et al 2008 Simple reaction time; code substitution; code substitution delayed; logical reasoning symbolic; mathematical processing; running memory; Sternberg memory recall
Ling et al 2009 Raven's Advanced Progressive Matrices; Memory Scanning task; Number-Pair Matching task; Sustained Attention task; Arrow Flanker task
Hammett et al 2010 Raven's Advanced Progressive Matrices; Backward Digit Span
Benton & Donohoe 2011 Word recall; reaction-time; vigilance rapid information processing task; Controlled Oral Word Association Test
Alves et al 2013 Forward Digit Span; Backward Digit Span; Mini-Mental State Examination; Stroop Test; Trail Making Test; Delay Recall Test
In total, these studies use <=31 distinct measures of cognitive functioning; this heterogeneity renders any summary very difficult as most measures were used in one and only one experiment. The Hemmings mood state inventory, random number generation task, forward verbal recall, & Uchida-Kraepelin serial calculation test are used in 2 experiments each, but the backward digit span is used in 4 experiments, and the RAPM is used in 4 experiments - so it would be best to analyze those two
# RAPM
## Data
I decided to code multiple relevant variables:
1. diet: if no diet was specified, assume omnivorousness since as little as 5% of Western population are vegetarians
- 0: omnivore
- 1: vegan or vegetarian
2. sleep:
- 0: no mention is made of sleep deprivation
- 1: if sleep deprivation in the experimental as opposed to control group
3. IQ:
- 0: RAPM
4. dose: total amount of creatine administered, in grams; average is total amount of creatine divided by number of days on which creatine is taken by a subject
5. age: average mean of all subjects' age; medians were treated as means if that was provided instead, and means given of range endpoints if only that was provided
6. type: creatine can be consumed in multiple forms.
[Creatine monohydrate](!Wikipedia) (CM) is the most common, but also used in a study is [creatine ethyl ester](!Wikipedia) (CEE). While CEE was developed to allow smaller doses than CM, [Katseres et al 2009](/docs/creatine/2009-katseres.pdf "Non-enzymatic hydrolysis of creatine ethyl ester") suggests it breaks down far too fast to be effective and so CEE doses may not be 1:1 equivalent with CM.
year study n.e mean.e sd.e n.c mean.c sd.c type dose total dose average age diet
---- ------------- --- ----- ---- --- ----- ---- ----------- ----------- ------------ -------- -----
2003 Rae 25 13.7 4.1 25 9.7 3.8 Monohydrate 210 5 25.59 1
2009 Ling 17 120.3 5.945 17 116.1 9.086 Ethyl ester 75 5 21 0
2010 Hammett 11 13.45 2.25 11 11.45 3.8 Monohydrate 110 15.7 27.59 0
## Results
The 3 studies turn in the expected statistically-significant positive result in the random-effects meta-analysis, due largely to Rae 2003:
~~~{.R}
Test for Heterogeneity:
Q(df = 2) = 1.1471, p-val = 0.5635
Model Results:
estimate se zval pval ci.lb ci.ub
0.7606 0.2017 3.7709 0.0002 0.3653 1.1559
~~~
![A forest plot of the 3 studies](/images/creatine/forest.png)
When vegetarianism is used as a covariate (this applies only to Rae 2003), the pooled Ling & Hammett estimate is then just barely statistically-significant:
~~~{.R}
Test of Moderators (coefficient(s) 2):
QM(df = 1) = 1.1256, p-val = 0.2887
Model Results:
estimate se zval pval ci.lb ci.ub
intrcpt 0.5661 0.2726 2.0767 0.0378 0.0318 1.1003
diet 0.4299 0.4052 1.0609 0.2887 -0.3643 1.2242
~~~
Unfortunately, with 3 studies I can't investigate dose meaningfully, nor try to check for publication bias. This leaves me quite skeptical: with more than a decade, have so few studies really been done, or are the proverbial filedrawers bulging with others I couldn't find?
<!-- library(pwr); pwr.t.test(n = sum(creatine$n.e), d = 0.57) -->
# Backward Digit Span
## Data
year study n.e mean.e sd.e n.c mean.c sd.c type dose total dose average age diet
---- ------------- --- ----- ---- --- ----- ---- ----------- ----------- ------------ -------- -----
2010 Hammett 11 11 Monohydrate 110 15.7 27.59 0
2003 Rae 25 8.5 1.76 25 7.05 1.19 Monohydrate 210 5 25.59 1
2013 Alves 25 3.75 1.0 22 3.25 0.76 Monohydrate 25 4.17 66.8 0
## Results
# Source
~~~{.R}
set.seed(7777) # for reproducible numbers
# TODO: factor out common parts of `png` (& make less square), and `rma` calls
library(XML)
creatine <- readHTMLTable(colClasses = c("integer", "character", rep("numeric", 11)),
"http://www.gwern.net/Creatine")[[2]]
# install.packages("metafor") # if not installed
library(metafor)
cat("Basic random-effects meta-analysis of all studies:\n")
res1 <- rma(measure="SMD", m1i = mean.e, m2i = mean.c, sd1i = sd.e, sd2i = sd.c, n1i = n.e, n2i = n.c,
data = creatine); res1
png(file="~/wiki/images/creatine/forest.png", width = 580, height = 580)
forest(res1, slab = paste(creatine$study, creatine$year, sep = ", "))
invisible(dev.off())
cat("Random-effects with vegetarian covariate:\n")
rma(measure="SMD", m1i = mean.e, m2i = mean.c, sd1i = sd.e, sd2i = sd.c, n1i = n.e, n2i = n.c,
data=creatine, mods = ~ diet)
system(paste('cd ~/wiki/images/creatine/ &&',
'for f in *.png; do convert "$f" -crop',
'`nice convert "$f" -virtual-pixel edge -blur 0x5 -fuzz 10% -trim -format',
'\'%wx%h%O\' info:` +repage "$f"; done'))
system("optipng -o9 -fix ~/wiki/images/creatine/*.png", ignore.stdout = TRUE)
~~~
# Study details
- Watanabe 2002, McMorris 2006, McMorris et al 2007, McMorris 2007, Rawson et al 2008, Benton & Donohoe 2011: excluded for not using a measure of intelligence.
- Gastner et al 2007: excluded for lack of necessary details.
## Rae 2003
[Rae et al 2003](http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC1691485/pdf/14561278.pdf "Oral creatine monohydrate supplementation improves brain performance: a double-blind, placebo-controlled, cross-over trial")
> In this work, we tested the hypothesis that oral creatine supplementation (5g daily for six weeks) would enhance intelligence test scores and working memory performance in 45 young adult, vegetarian subjects in a double-blind, placebo-controlled, cross-over design. Creatine supplementation had a significant positive effect (_p_=0.0001) on both working memory (backward digit span) and intelligence (Raven's Advanced Progressive Matrices), both tasks that require speed of processing.
> Forty-five vegan or vegetarian subjects (12 males (median age of 27.5, range of 19-37 years), 33 females (median age of 24.9, range of 18-40 years); 18 vegan (median duration of 4.6 years, range of 0.7-17 years) and 27 vegetarian (median duration of 14.3, range of 1-23 years)) were recruited with informed consent from among the student population of The University of Sydney
> The study followed a double-blind, placebo-controlled, cross-over design. Subjects were seen on four separate occasions, at six-week intervals, following an overnight fast to minimize any fluctuations in blood glucose.
> A cognitive test battery was also administered. At the end of the first and third test sessions, subjects were given an envelope marked with their study number and containing 5 g doses of supplement (creatine monohydrate ((2-methylguanido)acetic acid); Pan Pharmaceuticals, Australia) or placebo (maltodextrin; Manildra Starches, Australia) in plastic vials. Subjects were asked to consume this supplement at the same time each day for the next six weeks and received advice on how best to take this supplement to ensure maximum solubility and absorption. Subjects returned the envelope with unused vials at the end of each six-week period and the number of vials remaining was used to assess compliance, validated against increases in red cell (tissue) creatine. Between visits 2 and 3, the subjects consumed no supplement. Note: six weeks has been shown to be an adequate 'wash-out' period (Harris et al. 1992).
> Subjects completed timed (10 min) parallel versions of Raven's Advanced Progressive Matrices (RAPMs) constructed to have equal levels of difficulty based on the published normative performance data and verified by us on an independent sample of 20 subjects.
> Supplementation with oral creatine monohydrate significantly increased intelligence (as measured by RAPMs done under time pressure, figure 1a) compared with placebo (F3 ,33 = 32.3, p , 0.0001; repeated-measures ANOVA). There was no significant effect of treatment order (F1 ,33 = 1.62, p = 0.21), although there was a significant interaction with treatment order (F3 ,99 = 6.7, p = 0.0004). The mean RAPMs raw score under placebo was 9.7 (s.d. = 3.8) items correct in 10 min versus 13.7 (s.d. = 4.1) items correct under the experimental treatment. Supplementation with oral creatine monohydrate (figure 1b) significantly affected performance on BDS (F3 ,34 = 29.0, p , 0.0001), with no effect of order (F3 ,10 2 = 0.98, p = 0.40). Mean BDS under the placebo was 7.05 items (s.d. = 1.19), compared with a mean of 8.5 items under creatine treatment (s.d. = 1.76).
## Gastner et al 2007
"Use of creatine containing preparation e.g. for improving memory, retentivity, long-term memory and for preventing mental fatigue condition, comprising e.g. Ginkgo biloba, ginseng and niacin" ([English translation](http://www.google.com/patents/DE102007030495A1), [original](http://www.google.com/patents/DE102007030495A1?cl=de)): German patent filed in June 2007 by Dr. Thomas Gastner, Frauke Selzer, Dr. Hans-Peter, Dr. Bendikt Hammer, for [Alzchem Trostberg Gmbh](http://www.alzchem.com/):
> Use of a creatine containing preparation for improving memory, retentivity, long-term memory and for preventing mental fatigue conditions, comprising e.g. at least a further physiologically effective component of the series Ginkgo biloba, ginseng, taiga root, yam root, lecithin, choline, phosphatidylserine, dimethylamino ethanol, acetyl choline, acetyl-L-carnitine, glutathione, glutamine, cysteine, vitamin A, E, B1, B2, B6, B12, folic acid, pantothenic acid and/or zinc, is claimed. Use of a creatine-component containing preparation for improving memory, retentivity, long-term memory and for preventing mental fatigue conditions, comprising at least a further physiologically effective component of the series Ginkgo biloba, ginseng, taiga root, yam root, lecithin, choline, phosphatidylserine, dimethylamino ethanol, acetyl choline, acetyl-L-carnitine, glutathione, glutamine, cysteine, vitamin A, E, B1, B2, B6, B12, E, niacin, biotin, folic acid, pantothenic acid, zinc, manganese, selenium, magnesium, coenzyme Q10, glucose, colostrum, synephrine, octopamine, caffeine, theophylline, alpha -linolenic acid, eicosapentaenoic acid, omega-3-fatty acid, piracetam, aniracetam, memantine, pyritinol, galantamine, vinpocetin, pangamic acid and/or optionally organic or inorganic salts and/or optionally esters, is claimed.
Section 2, "Effectiveness":
> Subjects were divided randomly into four groups of 25 people each. The age of the subjects varied between 18 and 64 years. The four groups (a-d) were given twice per day for six weeks following each test substances in softgel capsules:
>
> a. placebo (1500 mg maltodextrin)
> b. creatine monohydrate (1500 mg)
> c. Ginkgo biloba leaves dry extract (120 mg)
> d. creatine monohydrate (1500 mg) and Ginkgo biloba leaves dry extract (120 mg)
It measured:
1. Backward Digit Span
2. Raven's Advanced Progressive Matrices
3. Uchida-Kraepelin test
But reported only pre and post-test scores, and not standard deviations; nor was any kind of statistical test reported, making it difficult to infer anything about the results.
It is unclear where this experiment was done, by whom, or whether it was ever published. (The "in press" citation to Mielcarz et al 2007 turns out to refer to McMorris et al 2007.) Nothing in the English translation indicates that it was published anywhere else; searches failed to find anything related to this but the patent itself; the Uchida-Kraepelin test is very unusual and I tried searching for anything relating to it and creatine in Google/Google-Scholar in English & German but turned up nothing besides discussions of Watanabe 2002.
On 23 September 2013, I attempted to reach Gastner via the [Alzchem contact form](http://www.alzchem.com/en/contacts). (Gastner knows English, as demonstrated by co-authoring "Creatine - its chemical synthesis, chemistry, and legal status" in _Creatine and Creatine Kinase in Health and Disease_.) 3 months later on 14 December 2013, I mailed a physical letter to the Trostberg, Germany address listed in _Creatine and Creatine Kinase_.
<!-- TODO: if no response via contact form, try mailing a physical letter to Germany: "Degussa AG, Dr. Albert-Frank-Straße 32, D-83308 Trostberg, Germany"; from _Creatine and Creatine Kinase_
If still no reply after a few months... are RAPM standard deviations sufficiently homogeneous to allow borrowing them from somewhere else...? -->
## Ling 2009
[Ling et al 2009](http://jtoomim.org/files/Ling_2009-Cognitive_effects_of_creatine_ethyl_ester_supplementation.pdf "Cognitive effects of creatine ethyl ester supplementation") saw the creatine group post-score 4 points higher than controls
> There were 34 participants (including 12 females) who completed the study, with a mean age of 21 years (SD: 1.38; range: 18-24). Participants were excluded, if they presented with a medical history of drug and/or alcohol abuse, diagnosed psychiatric disorders, diabetes, renal insufficiency (kidney dysfunction) or had recently or were currently supplementing with a creatine-based substance. None of the participants was vegetarian.
> The final task participants undertook was a modified version of Raven's Advanced Progressive Matrices (e.g. Raven et al., 1998) presented on a PC using [Macromedia Flash Player](http://iqtest.dk/main.swf). The difficulty of the 39 questions gradually increased and was constrained by a 40-min time limit.
> At the end of the first testing phase, participants were given a large envelope that contained 15 plastic vials of either 5 g doses of CEE (obtained through the online store Discount Supplements) or a placebo, maltodextrin (obtained from the manufacturer Chemical Nutrition; http://www.cnpprofessional.co.uk).
> There was a significant effect of test phase on performance in the IQ test [F(1,32) = 88.98, P < 0.01] with participants scoring a mean of 112 (SD: 9.44) at baseline, and 118 (7.89) at the end of the study. There was no significant main effect of supplement condition [F(1,32) = 0.56, NS]. However, the interaction was significant [F(1,32) = 81.18, P < 0.01]. Pairwise comparisons indicated that participants in the creatine condition performed worse than the placebo group in the first phase of testing, with baseline means for creatine group of 108 (SD: 7.42) and for placebo, 116 (SD: 9.60) (Tukey HSD, P < 0.01). Performance of the creatine group also improved significantly over the supplementation period, with the mean of 108 at baseline increasing to 120 (SD: 5.95) at the end of study (Tukey HSD, P < 0.01). Further pairwise comparisons indicated that there was no significant improvement in the performance of the placebo group over the supplementation period (P > 0.05).
> Performance of the creatine group also improved significantly over the supplementation period, with the mean of 108 at baseline increasing to 120 (SD: 5.95) at the end of study
Using the [spreadsheet of data Ling provided me](/docs/creatine/2009-ling-data.xls):
~~~{.R}
R> ling <- read.csv(stdin(),header=TRUE)
Creatine,IQ
1,120
1,118
1,126
1,121
1,119
1,118
1,125
1,117
1,133
1,116
1,124
1,114
1,110
1,130
1,123
1,116
1,115
2,110
2,112
2,105
2,106
2,115
2,105
2,112
2,124
2,119
2,133
2,116
2,123
2,122
2,110
2,125
2,105
2,131
R> summary(ling)
Creatine IQ
Min. :1.0 Min. :105
1st Qu.:1.0 1st Qu.:112
Median :1.5 Median :118
Mean :1.5 Mean :118
3rd Qu.:2.0 3rd Qu.:124
Max. :2.0 Max. :133
R> i <- ling[ling$Creatine==1,]$IQ; mean(i); sd(i)
[1] 120.3
[1] 5.945
R> i <- ling[ling$Creatine==2,]$IQ; mean(i); sd(i)
[1] 116.1
[1] 9.086
R> # we use a _t_-test rather than a Wilcoxon to replicate Ling's probable analysis
R> t.test(IQ ~ Creatine, data=ling)
Welch Two Sample t-test
data: IQ by Creatine
t = 1.608, df = 27.58, p-value = 0.1192
alternative hypothesis: true difference in means is not equal to 0
95% confidence interval:
-1.163 9.634
sample estimates:
mean in group 1 mean in group 2
120.3 116.1
~~~
## Hammett 2010
["Dietary supplementation of creatine monohydrate reduces the human fMRI BOLD signal"](http://www.researchgate.net/publication/44694983_Dietary_supplementation_of_creatine_monohydrate_reduces_the_human_fMRI_BOLD_signal/file/9fcfd50bb2b9e2fba1.pdf), Hammett et al 2010; quotes relevant for calculating the variables:
> To establish whether the magnitude of the BOLD response is influenced by Cr levels, we have measured responses to visual stimuli in the primary visual cortex (V1) of 22 healthy human volunteers using fMRI, before and after oral administration of Cr or a placebo (11 in the Cr group and 11 in the placebo group).
> The mean and median age of the Cr group was 30.18 and 27 years (SD = 8.37) respectively and the mean and median age of the placebo group was 25 years (SD = 4.82).
> Creatine supplementation (Sci-Mx: Gloucestershire, UK) was provided at a dose of 20 g/day for five days, followed by two additional days at a dose of 5 g/day.
> In order to verify previous reports of cognitive enhancement following Cr supplementation we also measured performance on the Backwards Digit Span (BDS) [28] and Raven's Advanced Progressive Matrices (RAPM) [24] prior to each scan. The BDS comprises a set of number sequences of increasing length with two different sequences of each length. Subjects were required to repeat each sequence backwards. The test was terminated when the subject failed to repeat two sequences of the same length. Different number sequences were used for the two testing sessions. Subjects were required to complete as many items of the RAPM as possible in 5 min. Since the RAPM tests are ordered in terms of difficulty, odd-numbered and even-numbered tests were administered on weeks 1 and 2 respectively.
> Performance on the RAPM increased non-significantly by 9.6% following Cr (t = 1.882, df = 10, p = 0.0745) and reduced non-significantly by 4.5% (t = 0.7733, df = 10, p = 0.4572) following placebo. A Group × Week ANOVA revealed a main effect of week (F(1, 20) = 5.75, p = 0.026, two-tailed) and a significant interaction between week and compound (F(1, 20) = 8.58, p = 0.008, two-tailed) for BDS performance. No significant effects were found for RAPM performance.
What's the standard deviation which produces a p-value of 0.0745 on an increase of 9.6% & a sample size of 11 in each group? Hard to tell, but Hammett provided me the pre/post scores:
pre post
------------- ------------ -------- ---------
creatine mean 12.27 13.45
SD 3.31 2.25
placebo mean 12 11.45
SD 3.52 3.8
## Alves et al 2013
[Alves data table](http://www.plosone.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pone.0076301)
> The CR and CR+ST groups received 20 g of creatine monohydrate (4 x 5 g/d) for five days followed by 5 g/d as a single dose throughout the trial.
Alves et al 2013: combined the placebo & placebo+strength-training groups, and the creatine & creatine+strength-training groups
<!--
placebo: n=12 3.3 ± 0.8
placebo+training: n=10 3.2 ± 0.7
10+12=22
((3.3*12)+(3.2*10))/22=3.25
(((12-1) * 0.8) + ((10-1)*0.7))/(22-2)=0.76
creatine: n=13 3.7 ± 0.9
creatine+training: n=12 3.8 ± 1.1
13+12=25
((3.7*13)+(3.8*12))/25=3.75
(((13-1) * 0.9) + ((12-1) * 1.1))/(25-2)=1.0
-->